Monday, June 16, 2025

Harris Friedman and the Science of Transpersonal Psychology

I'm deep in my final paper for my independent study, so once again I'm posting part of a writeup for the research I've done along the way. Harris Friedman and the Science of Transpersonal Psychology © 2025 by Kimberly Israel is licensed under CC BY-NC 4.0

Romanticism, Scientism, and Constructivism

Harris Friedman advocates for transpersonal psychology to be circumscribed as a scientific field, suggesting that nonscientific approaches could legitimately belong to “transpersonal studies” but not “transpersonal psychology” (Friedman, 2002). He argues that psychology is generally treated as a science, that transpersonal psychology was originally developed along those scientific lines, and that using the word “psychology” to define the field carries an expectation of scientific methodology, particularly in clinical settings. He describes three major attitudes that diminish the scientific reputation of the field: romanticism, scientism, and constructivism.

 

Friedman (2005) describes romanticism as “rejection of rationality, fascination for the so-called exotic, erosion of all basis for discriminating among truth claims, fixation of idyllic images regarding humanity's relationship with nature, and attributions of supernatural claims without evidentiary support.” Examples include using astrology in a clinical practice or interpreting Eastern religious concepts as corresponding to particular states of consciousness (Friedman, 2002). Indeed, he demonstrates the issue quite succinctly in a non-paper in which he proposes to describe “all cogent scientific conceptualizations of the non-dual” (Friedman, 2018a) and presents the reader with a blank page. The abstract to the “article” explains that characterization of psychological states as non-dual depends on metaphysical constructs, which cannot be scientifically evaluated.

 

Friedman (2002) goes on to say that scientism is more of a concern outside the field than within; it is characterized by a narrow conception of what can and should be studied using scientific methods and frequently carries the assumption that romanticism is all transpersonal psychology has to offer. Transpersonal psychology is practically taboo in conventional psychology departments (Friedman, 2018b), possibly due to scientistic prejudice.

 

Constructivism, meanwhile, is the extreme postmodern view that because all human knowledge-building is influenced by culture and subject to bias and blind spots, no meaningful concept of reality is possible. While recognition of the limits of objectivity is certainly useful, science cannot proceed without some sense that methodology matters, that evidence means something beyond personal preference (Friedman, 2002).

 

Of course, much of the subject matter important to transpersonal psychology is outside the realm of science: transcendence, divinity, and nonduality are metaphysical concepts that cannot be directly probed. Science can, however, explore areas such as the effect of subjectively transcendent experiences on people’s lives or the influence of culture on one’s concept of transcendence. We can study the “phenomena,” the material available to our senses, including reports of others’ experience, without claiming access to the “noumena,” the things in themselves, such as the ontological validity of subjective experience (Friedman, 2002). Although Friedman takes a strong stance in favor of transpersonal psychology as a science, he also accepts that nonscientific sources, such as spiritual teachers or traditions, may well be valuable sources of knowledge in one’s personal life. Although Friedman is a psychologist rather than a paleontologist, his stance seems to approximate Gould’s (1997) position of “nonoverlapping magisteria.”


Western Romanticism Towards Aikido


As a practitioner of aikido, Friedman (2005) uses it as a case study of the kind of romanticism he finds objectionable in the transpersonal field. He begins by noting that the roots of the martial art are in the training needed for a disarmed samurai to have a chance of surviving against his opponents long enough to escape. The character traits of courage and present-moment focus emphasized by the practice would also have been specifically adaptive in combat conditions.

 

Friedman notes that in cross-cultural situations, Westerners interested in transpersonal systems frequently romanticize traditional lines of authority and teaching styles, assuming without evidence that because the tradition demonstrates some benefits, it is correct in all things. As an example, he refers to the primary global school of aikido, leadership of which has been handed down from father to son. The current head of the school is the grandson of the man who developed much of modern aikido but is young and inexperienced. Friedman sees unquestioning acceptance of the current leader’s qualifications as a form of romanticism.

 

He also challenges the tendency to romanticize Japan as the spiritual home or truest expression of aikido simply because the practice developed there: today, aikido has many more practitioners outside Japan, where baseball is far more popular. Finally, he explains that the effectiveness of aikido can be understood without reference to the supernatural. The martial art does take a transpersonal perspective of viewing self and opponent as one system, and Friedman argues that doing so results in greater attunement to the opponent and a more effective defense. Impressive feats like the “unbendable arm” don’t rely on a conceptually nebulous “energy” but on physiological relaxation of opposing muscles and the prevention of inhibitory nerve impulses through clarity of intent (Friedman, 2005).


A middle-aged man wearing a gi with a black hakama spars with a younger man wearing a gi. The younger man has his feet and one hand on the floor; the other arm is raised. The older man is standing, leaning towards the younger, with one hand on the palm and one on the shoulder of the raised arm
Image credit: Christelle Fillonneau

https://commons.wikimedia.org/wiki/File:Nikyo_omote.jpeg


(This is not an image of Friedman, just a representative image of people practicing aikido)







                                                                        References


Friedman, H. (2002). Transpersonal psychology as a scientific field. International Journal of Transpersonal Studies, 21(1), 175-187.

Friedman, H. (2005). Problems of romanticism in transpersonal psychology: a case study of aikido. The Humanistic Psychologist, 33(1), 3-24.

Friedman, H.L. (2018a). An explication of all cogent scientific conceptualizations regarding the non-dual: finding nothing to write. International Journal of Transpersonal Studies, 37(2), 116-118.

Friedman, H.L. (2018b). Transpersonal psychology as a heterodox approach to psychological science: focus on the construct of self-expansiveness and its measure. Archives of Scientific Psychology 6, 230-242.

Gould, S.J. (1997). Nonoverlapping magisteria. Natural History, 106, 16-22.

Friday, June 6, 2025

Buriro, Z. A., Nawaz, H. I., & Asif, M. (2025). FROM EMPHASIS TO MOCKERY: THE MULTIFUNCTIONAL ROLE OF REDUPLICATION IN SINDHI. Journal of Applied Linguistics and TESOL (JALT), 8(2), 959-969.

 What They Did

The researchers studied the linguistic role of word and sound reduplication in Sindhi, a major language of Pakistan, particularly the province of Sindh. They collected 31 instances of reduplication from social settings and analyzed their contextual meaning with the assistance of native speakers. Reduplication of full words often carries emotional content. Saying گهم گهم (‘ghum ghum’) is a reference to wandering around and can be used affectionately in the sense of a parent telling a child to go have fun. The phrase کل کل (‘khil khil’) can refer to someone laughing happily, but in response to mocking, the meaning is more akin to “You’re laughing now, but my time will come.”

Sound reduplications may be rhymes, near rhymes, or alliterations. They frequently carry information about the situational context. Saying وٺ سٺ (‘wath sath’) refers to being in a hurry, particularly for handling complex plans such as wedding preparations. The phrase پويتياگتي (‘agte poete’) literally means “back and forth” and socially refers to a situation in which another person won’t make a commitment. When people say سهانگو مهانگ (‘mahango sahango’), which translates as “costly but necessary,” it’s generally a comment on rising costs of necessary items and the fact that they have to be purchased anyway.

Sinhi speakers also use reduplication of words that have no literal meaning. The phrase هور هور (‘horr horr’) is used to mock someone in an exaggerated way, while تر تر (‘tur tur’) is an angry warning that the opponent in a confrontation had better leave.

 

Further Exploration

I use reduplication in my own communication. My spouse and I often want to text “k” as a shorthand for “Ok” but somehow “k” on its own feels abrupt or dismissive, so we’ve taken to using “kk.” This defeats the purpose of shortening “Ok,” but it’s also developed a different feeling: “kk” carries a sense of being on the same wavelength, of being in an activity together, even if we’re carrying out different tasks. “Ok,” on the other hand, has a sense of accepting information or instruction from the other person.

Similarly, if I text “neat neat” in response to a story or observation, that means I’m engaged and open to hearing more, whereas “neat” without the repetition has more of a sense of “I’m happy for you and want to be supportive but am not necessarily interested for myself.” Of course, these examples are highly idiosyncratic, but many other examples of reduplication in English will likely be familiar (see https://daily.jstor.org/the-nitty-gritty-on-reduplication-so-good-you-have-to-say-it-twice/).

In the sentence “I’ll make the tuna salad and you make the salad-salad,” the repetition indicates that the second salad is closer to the central concept of a salad. Sound reduplications in English are often informal and nonliteral. The phrase “loosey-goosey,” for example, seems to have a sense of being unstructured almost to the point of falling apart, which may indicate negligence or flexibility, depending on the situation, but has nothing to do with geese. Lots of other languages also use reduplication, but that’s a rabbit hole for another day!

Flag of Sindh, white line drawing on dark green background. Grains and vegetables over wavy lines, surrounded by a pointed oval shape nested in leafy branches, topped with a star containing three dots, all resting on a crescent with the horns pointing up, on whch words are written in an Arabic script
Image credit: Baba66

Monday, June 2, 2025

Hunter, Holly, Grace Blackburn, Benjamin J. Ashton, and Amanda R. Ridley. "Group size affects spontaneous quantity discrimination performance in wild Western Australian magpies (Gymnorhina tibicen dorsalis)." Animal Cognition 28, no. 1 (2025):

What They Did

The researchers studied the ability of wild Western Australian magpies to distinguish quantities. The magpies live in groups of 3 to 15 in urban semi-natural areas and are accustomed to humans. Individual magpies were separated from their group, either by waiting for them to wander off naturally or by distracting the other group members. The bird under study was then presented with a randomized sequence of choices between two boards: one with two strips of cheese and the other with two, three, four, or five strips. Over the course of the experiment, 42 birds were tested, each being offered the four choices 15 times on separate days.

 The researchers found that the birds chose the larger amount of food more than half the time, with better performance correlated with greater differences between the amounts. They also found that birds that lived in smaller groups were more likely to choose five strips of cheese over two, but the group size had no effect for the other combinations. The correlation between “correct” choice likelihood and difference between the choices suggests that the birds decide based on ratio rather than absolute number, with the smallest ratio (i.e. 2:5) easiest to detect.

The researchers also propose two hypotheses for the effect of group size. Smaller groups may offer less protection in intergroup conflicts, making it important to compare the size of rival group. Smaller groups also have fewer individuals watching for predators, so it’s more important to maximize the benefit of limited foraging time.

 

Further Exploration

The researchers describe two cognitive systems animals use to process quantity information: the object-file system tracks each object and works for small numbers, while the approximate number system provides a general idea of which group is larger. This seems to be the case for humans too. Automatically knowing how many objects are in a group in called subitizing (see https://hechingerreport.org/proof-points-subitizing/). Perceptual subitizing means seeing the number of objects “all at once.” For humans, the maximum is typically 4 to 6 objects.

Larger numbers, however, can be subitized by mentally dividing the array into groups and subitizing the number in each group, such as seeing three groups of four and knowing there are 12 objects without thinking about counting or multiplication. This process is called conceptual subitizing. Subitizing sounds as if it might be analogous to the object-file system described in animal cognition, and of course humans also seem to have an approximate number system. We can tell which group of items contains more, and we’re better at it if there’s a greater difference between them.

Humans can also count one by one, as long as we have the patience for. A lot of animals deal with quantity in a lot of different ways (see https://www.quantamagazine.org/animals-can-count-and-use-zero-how-far-does-their-number-sense-go-20210809/), but I haven’t seen any evidence that they can count symbolically and indefinitely the way humans do. It would also be interesting to see how animals handle number when the objects are dissimilar or when distractor objects are included, but that’s a rabbit hole for another day!    

A Western Australian magpie on the ground facing away from the camera. The front is all black, whilte the back, wings, and tail have large areas of both black and white
Image credit: D. Gordon E. Robertson

Harris Friedman and the Science of Transpersonal Psychology

I'm deep in my final paper for my independent study, so once again I'm posting part of a writeup for the research I've done alon...